Ana səhifə

Grounding in computer-supported collaborative problem solving


Yüklə 1.41 Mb.
səhifə13/18
tarix25.06.2016
ölçüsü1.41 Mb.
1   ...   10   11   12   13   14   15   16   17   18

Structure of grounding patterns


The statistics presented so far summarize grounding patterns across protocols by counting pairs of utterances. However, these pairs of utterances form more complex patterns and episodes. To describe larger patterns, we first discuss turn taking, then present different forms of irregular forms of turn taking: simultaneous turns and parallel threads.
      1. Turn taking


The mechanisms of turn taking in the MOO are very different than in voice conversation. On one hand, there is no constraint to wait for one's partner answer before to say more. On the other hand, one can implicitly or explicitly refer to utterances earlier than the last one, since they are still visible on the screen. Hence a MOO conversation between two people is not a simple alternation of turns. We counted the index of complexity on 'say' and 'page' commands. This index evaluates the regularity of turn taking. Its value would be zero if, knowing the speaker at turn n we have a probability of 1 for predicting who will speak at n+1. Its value would be 1 if knowing the speaker at turn n does not give us any information regarding who will speak at n+1. Its average value in our protocols is 0.9 (SD = .06), i.e. it indicates a complete non-systematicity of turn taking!

Interestingly, the average index of complexity is exactly the same if we consider the group with a high acknowledgment rate and the group with a low acknowledgment rate (both 0.9 as well). This seems to indicate that irregular turn taking does not really affect acknowledgment. It may imply that acknowledgment has not to be systematic, an utterance being acknowledged only when there is a need for acknowledging it. This point contribute for our model of grounding (see section 7) in which the need for acknowledging a piece of information constitutes a key parameter for predicting grounding efforts.

In typed interactions, the absence of intonation for indicating the end of a turn is probably compensated by the fact that sending the message indicates the end of a turn. It occurs that an accidental 'return' sends a message before it is finished and this message is hence hence acknowledged prematurely, as in example 40.

95.5

Lobby

H

page Sherlock Let's try to see systematically who has no alibi between




95.7

r6

S

' ok

96.3

Lobby

H

page Sherlock ... between 21.00 et 22.30. Me I start from the beginning of our notebooks and you from the en, ok?

Example 40: Accidental 'return' leads to premature acknowledgment (from Pair 19, translated)

Sometimes, a speaker wants to express himself with several turns, i.e. to keep the floor. He could send a long message, but there are several reasons for not doing it: (1) explanations are clearer with a sequence of small messages (lines) than as a big message; (2) the text entry pane in the MOO window generally displays a few lines, 3 in these experiments, and scrolling makes difficult the edition of longer messages; and (3) it is sometimes desirable to keep the listener attention during the time necessary for entering the whole message (for instance if one types a long message with the 'say' command, the listener may leave the room in between and hence not receive the message). In examples 41, the speaker implicitly indicates that he wants keep the floor by using a ":".



110.3

Bar

S

say so here is my idea:




110.7

Bar

S

say i suspect rolf AND Claire Loretan

Example 41: Implicit way to keep the floor (from Pair 3)

Conversely, sending the message may not be enough for triggering change of speaker, as in example 41 above ("ok'" at 96.3) or in example 42 below. In this pair, Hercule had problems26 for using the MOO and namely for communicating. Sherlock explicitly indicates the end of his turn as in radio communication (106.3 and 106.4), thereby inviting Hercule to react to his previous messages.



105

K

S

say there is a contract which says that the painting is insured for 10000000000000 francs for Oscar's benefice




105.3

K

S

say and mona-lisa signed it too

105.5

K

S

say so they must know each other

105.9

K

S

say but oscar talk about mona-lisa as a customer

106

K

S

say so it's strange

106.2

K

S

say but i dont know anything else about that

106.3

K

S

say stop

106.4

K

S

say over

Example 42: Explicit hand over messages 'stop' and 'over' (from Pair 3, line 105.5 translated)
      1. Simultaneous turns


In typed interaction, two users can 'talk' simultaneously without disturbing each other. There are numerous examples of simultaneous speech in our protocoles. In the example 43, the two subjects simultaneously read their notebook which lead them to reject simultaneously (88.2) Sherlock's initial proposal. Later (88.9), they simultaneously complete Skerlock's list of remaining suspects (88.7).

87.1

r3

S

She couldn't have stolen the gun, could she?




87.4

r3

S

read giuzeppe from dn1

87.5

r3

H

I'm just checking something.

87.7

r3

H

read Giuzeppe from dn2

88.2

r3

S

No - Monda was in the restaurant till 9.

88.2

r3

H

No, she left around 9:00. She couldn't have stolen the gun.

88.7

r3

S

So Lisa, Rolf, Claire, Giuzeppe and Jacques are still open.

88.9

r3

S

and Oscar

88.9

r3

H

And Oscar...

Example 43: Simultaneous talk indicating parallel cognitive processes (from Pair 18)

In example 43, simultaneity of talk probably indicates that the two detectives perform in parallel similar cognitive processes, respectively rejecting and completing an hypothesis, on the same object, respectively utterances at 87.1 and 88.7. We do of course also observe simultaneous talk corresponding to independent cognitive processes like in example 44. However, although we did not systematically count all cases of simultaneous talk, in the majority of them, the two subjects say more or less the same thing or at least talk about the same thing.



97.9

K

S

' i'm reading the dn to make a synthesis. we just have to figure out who has a motive, had time to kill and could manipulate a weapon.




97.9

Priv

H

page sher we don't know yet who made that phonecall from here at 8:28

Example 44: Simultaneous talk indicating independent cognitive processes (from Pair 10 )

For the subject themselves, synchronous talk can be interpreted as an indicator that they partner his following them closely. In example 45, Hercule realizes this simultaneous thinking (76.3).



75.2

Bar

S

page her yes! I am sure now, I go interview someone we forget!!!!




75.4

Bar

S

page her marie!

75.5

r7

H

page sher by the way, I just realized that we forgot Marie

76.3

r7

H

page sher ask her, she might have seen him sneeking around the corner to get the colonel's weapon and Hans jacket. By the way, it's funny, we seem to have realized at the same second that we forgot about this stupid Marie.

Example 45: Hercule notices simultaneous talk (from Pair 13)

In the example 45 (above), the two messages about Marie are not exactly synchronous. The actual delay between the first and the second is 8 seconds. We have to take into account that the time indicated in protocols (log files) refers to the moment where the user sends his message (i.e. hits the 'return' key). Hercule started to type his sentence before Sherlock sent his message, they have been typing simultaneously but Sherlock finished his message and sent it 8 seconds before Hercule. When the user introduces a MOO command, (s)he usually watches the text entry zone which is located at the bottom of the MOO window and may not see the messages of his/her partner which are displayed a few lines above. If (s)he does, the incoming message may make irrelevant the message being typed and hence lead the subject to cancel it. We cannot quantify this process from our log files, since no MOO command is performed, we can only infer such phenomena in cases such as the examples 46 and 47.



71.1

r1

S

page her I suppose that the murder weapon is the gun but perhaps it is another object. What do you think about it?




71.7

r3

H

page s ok .This is waht I was writing to you.

Example 46: Hercule cancels his message (71.7) because of simultaneous talk (from Pair 20, translated )

161.8

Bar

H

' OK, he is still in love!




162.3

Bar

H

' and he could not get the gun

162.5

Bar

S

' I was on point to say that

Example 47: Sherlock cancels his message (162.5) because of simultaneous talk (from Pair 16)

We do also observe cases of synchronous interactions on the whiteboard, and especially the case which are interesting with respect to shared understanding: when the two subjects produce the same note as the same time like in example 48.



153.2

r4

H

say don't you think that heidi is innocent..she was with Lucie




154.4

r4

S

it means that both are innocent

154.9

r4

H

say ok colleague

155.3

r4

S

draw blue text 303 158 "H. ZELLER"

155.3

r4

H

draw blue text 271 157 "H.ZELLER"

155.4

r4

H

delete 59 [59 is the note written by Hercule]

Example 48: Synchronous writing on the whiteboard (from Pair 22)
      1. Interwoven turns


An acknowledgment in the MOO does not always refer to the last turn of the other speaker. This phenomena disturbs new MOO users. This is very often the case when more than 2 people interact, but it did also frequently occur in our experiments with only two subjects. In example 49, the pattern includes two interwoven acknowledgment patterns: H:88.5-S:89.3 and S:88.8-H90.3. In example 50, the pattern S43.6-H43.9 is indented inside the pattern H:43.5-S:44.1. It is the semi-persistence of talk in the MOO which enables subjects to cope with such situations.

88.5

r1

H

page sherlock but what about the gun?




88.8

Priv

S

'Hercule which motive jealousy? He would have killed hans no?

89.3

Priv

S

'Hercule he stole it when the colonel was in the bar

90.3

r1

H

page sherlock Giuzeppe wanted to avoid that one discovers that the painting was fake.

Example 49: XYXY turns (from Pair 11, translated )

43.5

Bar

H

Why does Heidi have a motive ?




43.6

Bar

S

How do you propose we should go further?

43.9

Bar

H

Should we merge our note books?

44.1

Bar

S

She said that she didn't like her (and Hans)

Example 50: XYYX turns (from Pair 12 )

Sometimes however, the speaker makes the effort of pointing to which utterance he refers to; in example 51, the reference is established by repeating a part of it of the acknowledged utterance..



82.9

r3

H

Ok, so it's not Lucie or Heidi, right.




83.1

r3

S

Well, lisa *showed* mona the painting was a fake. So whoever was in the bar the day before would also have known.

83.4

r3

S

Right - not Lucie or Heidi.

Example 51: In 83.4, "not Lucie or Heidi " makes explicit which utterance is acknowledged by "Right"(from Pair 18)

The MOO could be designed to support 'acknowledgment with reference' as in utterance 83.4 of the last example. We implemented a dialogue room in which the user could specify which utterances he was answering to. We did not further develop this approach but consider it as a promising way for future research.


      1. Parallel threads of conversation


The notion of interwoven turns may sound pejorative. We prefer to emphasize the subject skill to maintain two conversations in parallel. In some cases, subject S1 talks about topic T1 and subject S1 about topic T2, without any acknowledgment. This is probably the worst case with respect to the elaboration of a shared solution. However, it is not certain that the absence of acknowledgment means that one detective is not following the other's work in the background, but does not find useful to comment his partner work and hence simply report his own work. Our data do not enable us to study the cognitive consequence of such 'parallel and independent' talk.

More interesting are the cases of pairs who conduct together two different conversations at the same time, each conversation being acknowledged as a normal conversation, like in example 50 (above). In example 51p, Hercule is talking in the MOO about the Helmut Von Schneider and simultaneously taking notes in the whiteboard about Oscar Salève.



60.6

K

H

' would you agree that the gun must have been taken out of colonels room bewteen 8 and 9 pm?




60.9

r5

S

'Hercule why?

61

K

H

draw black arrow 283 187 313 204 [put first part of a cross in row Oscar Salève, column 'opportunity to take gun']

61

K

H

draw black arrow 308 190 286 206 [put first part of a cross in row Oscar Salève, column 'opportunity to kill']

61.5

K

H

' the colonel left his room for this period to have a drink

61.6

K

H

draw black arrow 370 188 370 188 put second part ofa cross in cell [put second part of a cross in row Oscar Salève, column 'opportunity to take gun']

61.7

K

H

draw black arrow 371 191 371 191 [put first part of a cross in row Oscar Salève, column 'opportunity to take kill']

Example 51: Parallel threads across modes: talking about one thing the MOO and another thing on the whiteboard (from Pair 12).

These results open a new avenue for research which was not originally included in this project. They raise two questions: (1) how newcomers to MOO environments progressively adapt themselves to models of dialogue which are very different from the usual voice conversation, (2) how models of dialogue in which several lines of argument may be pursued simultaneously affect problem solving. We would postulate that users take the best benefit from MOO interactions if they do not stick rigidly to voice dialogue patterns but learn how to use the more complex dialogue patterns which emerge in MOO conversations.


1   ...   10   11   12   13   14   15   16   17   18


Verilənlər bazası müəlliflik hüququ ilə müdafiə olunur ©atelim.com 2016
rəhbərliyinə müraciət