Ana səhifə

Casr report series


Yüklə 0.94 Mb.
səhifə9/16
tarix24.06.2016
ölçüsü0.94 Mb.
1   ...   5   6   7   8   9   10   11   12   ...   16

Table 5.12


Sleep issues, sleepiness, fatigue, and fatigue management


Author

Type

Availability

Research

Findings

Barr, Popkin, & Howarth (2009)

Report

Public

A review of recent developments in mathematical modelling and vehicle-based operator alertness monitoring technologies.

Reviews and discusses current activities with regard to the development of unobtrusive, in-vehicle, and real-time drowsy driver detection and fatigue monitoring/alerting systems.

Swann (2002)

Conference paper

Public

Reviews the issues of drugs, alcohol, and fatigue in heavy vehicle safety.

Truck drivers use stimulants for occupational reasons.

Drivers with sleep disordered breathing have an increased risk of an accident.

16% of heavy vehicle drivers have both sleep disordered breathing and symptoms of excessive daytime sleepiness, both of which can be successfully treated.


Mahon & Cross (2000)

Conference paper

Public

Outlines the findings of a pilot study of the FMP trialled in Queensland as an alternative to existing prescriptive approaches. The FMP requires heavy vehicle operators to have rostering and scheduling practices that consider fatigue-relevant issues in order to achieve accreditation.

Government prescriptive approaches to fatigue management lack clarity and can be confusing.

A number of benefits were associated with the FMP:

Increased awareness of fatigue issues and management/prevention strategies.

Improved lifestyle

Reductions in the frequency of fatigue symptoms and the use of negative coping strategies.

Numerous business benefits including reductions in accidents and injuries, improved staff morale, and improved management and productivity.



Anund, Kecklund, Vadeby, Hajlmdahl, & Akerstedt (2008)

Journal article

Public

Reports the findings of an experiment utilising a moving base simulator to determine the effects of milled rumble strips on driver fatigue. Rumble strips were simulated for both the edge line and centreline; 4 different designs of rumble strips were used.

Results showed an increase in sleepiness indicators prior to hitting the rumble strip, and an alerting effect after hitting the strip.

The observed alertness effect was short lived and signs of sleepiness returned in 5 minutes following the hitting of the rumble strip.



Hanowski, Hickman, Olson, & Bocanegra (2009)

Journal article

Public

Evaluates the impact of an additional driving hour (increase from 10 to 11 hours) on the critical incident (crash or near miss) involvement of truck drivers. Data was collected as part of a naturalistic truck driving study.

Analyses found an elevated risk of critical incident involvement in the first hour of driving, but no consistent significant differences between hours 2 through 11.

Analysis of time of day of critical incident involvement identified a strong positive correlation to national traffic density data.

This study found that there was no increased risk of experiencing a critical incident from truck drivers driving in the 11th hour compared to the 10th or any other hour.


National Transport Commission (2006)

NTC technical report

Public

Presents a summary of fatigue management information that has been used in the development of advanced fatigue management (AFM) option policy.

Presents detailed accounts of fatigue management programs and research studies relevant to AFM.

Economic Associates Pty Ltd (2003)

NTC regulatory impact statement

Public

Examines the impacts of regulations and code of practice to manage fatigue in heavy vehicle drivers.

A weakness of the regulatory scheme is a focus on drivers’ hours of work rather than the causes of fatigue.

Regulated working hours may be inadequate for the following reasons:

Prescribed minimum breaks may be inadequate;

Requirements for short breaks are too rigid;

Regulations are inflexible;

TFMS provides scheduling flexibility for, but places few fatigue management obligations on, employers;

Regulations do not recognise the need for the active management of fatigue;

The focus of enforcement remains on drivers rather than those who influence or make scheduling decisions.

The paper also outlines compliance options, standard hours, BFM, AFM, chain of responsibility, code of practice, work diaries and record keeping, and enforcement aspects of the proposed changes to the regulations.


Williamson, Friswell, & Feyer (2004)

NTC research report

Public

Compares the impact of day and night shift rosters on the fatigue and performance of heavy vehicle drivers.

Night shifts made drivers feel more tired than day shifts, but did not produce significantly poorer performance, indicating that night shift drivers are able to adequately manage their fatigue.

Williamson, Sadural, Feyer, & Friswell (2001)

Information paper

Public

An Australian survey of 1007 long distance road transport drivers

Drivers reported fatigue less often than they had in the previous survey.

Most drivers reported that they experienced fatigue in the first 10 hours of driving.

Drivers experienced fatigue most often during the early morning and to a lesser extent in the early afternoon.

Factors that drivers identified as increasing fatigue included long driving hours and problems associated with loading and unloading (particularly delays).

Strategies drivers reported as most effective for managing fatigue include: sleep, rest, drinks containing caffeine, and “stay-awake” drugs.

Fewer drivers in the current survey reported using “stay-awake” drugs.

Owner-drivers do longer trips but appear to have greater flexibility over trip scheduling.

Fatigue-related incidents are common occurrences for long distance drivers.

Drivers most commonly broke working hours regulations due to work organisational and reward factors.

Drivers paid in terms of the amount of work they did reported more fatigue than drivers paid at an hourly rate.

The survey demonstrated little change in the working conditions of long distance truck drivers between 1991 and 2001 although awareness of fatigue appears to have improved and the occurrence of fatigue has reduced.


Feyer, Williamson, Friswell, & Sadural (2001)

Information paper

Public

A survey of 200 Australian transport companies regarding knowledge, awareness, and management of fatigue.

The majority of companies reported that awareness of fatigue has increased over the past 5 years, however this increased awareness did not guarantee better management.

Half of the companies surveyed reported that fatigue was well managed and 20% reported that it was badly managed. It appears that drivers knowledge and awareness of fatigue issues is much better than the companies that employ them.

The majority of companies reported having considerable control over work schedules with strict estimated times of arrival being uncommon.

Companies reported less intervention and active management of fatigue for non-employee drivers.

The survey suggests that there is considerable scope for improving the understanding and management of fatigue in the industry.


AMR Interactive (2007)

NTC research report

Public

A survey of 613 heavy vehicle drivers regarding fatigue and its effects on drivers. Results of this survey are compared to earlier surveys of the same issues undertaken in 1991 and 1998.

75% of drivers view fatigue as a significant problem in the road freight industry and many drivers believe fatigue is not well managed within the industry.

While many drivers report experiencing fatigue related issues when driving, very few drivers consider fatigue to be more than a minor problem for them.

Long working hours was considered one of the most important contributors to fatigue. Other factors included irregular or inadequate sleep and other aspects of work such as having to stick to regulations, and heavy traffic.

40% of drivers reported occasionally driving contrary to regulations.

Driving without taking breaks was influenced by driving schedules and conditions, and the drivers’ motivations to make money.

Common reasons for not stopping for a break included scheduling and practical (e.g., nowhere to stop the truck) limitations.



AMR Interactive

NTC research report

Public

A survey of 314 heavy vehicle freight companies regarding attitudes towards and knowledge of fatigue.

A number of changes were observed with regard to attitudes, knowledge, and practices observed in 1998:

Perceptions that fatigue is well managed in the industry have increased.

Improvements in the implementation of formal fatigue and medical policies to include subcontractors, etc.

Perceptions of increased awareness of driver fatigue within the industry were lower than those observed in 1998.

Improvements in knowledge regarding causes of fatigue and effective fatigue management strategies.

Improvements in the determination of trip times.



Warner & Talko (2010)

Journal article

Public

An overview of a draft performance-based specification for heavy vehicle driver fatigue monitoring systems to enable the use of electronic work diaries (EWDs).

Authorities are yet to approve EWDs due to the ambiguous provisions within the HVDF legislation.

Enabling the use of EWDs will present stakeholders with numerous opportunities in other areas such as the generation of management reports or the introduction of non-roadside enforcement practices.



Brewer, Camilleri, & Zapanta (2010)

Conference paper

Public

Describes a rest stop provision strategy implemented in conjunction with new heavy vehicle fatigue management regulations.

Outlines the criteria for the selection of locations and the facilities provided at rest areas.

Cleaver, Simpson, de Roos, Hendry, & Peden (2009)

Conference paper

Public

Outlines the use of blue reflectors to indicate the location of informal rest areas for truck drivers.

The reflectors also provide a reminder to truck drivers of their obligations to manage fatigue.

The blue reflectors are used in a number of Australian states and are recognised by the heavy vehicle industry.



Baas, Charlton, & Bastin (2000)

Conference paper

Public

An evaluation of compliance with driving hours regulations and fatigue.

A sizeable number of drivers exceeded allowable driving hours.

High levels of fatigue and sleepiness were also observed.



Johns, 2000

Journal article

Public

A sleep physiologist studied sleep-related factors and drowsy driving. He suggested factors which may determine the likelihood of someone falling asleep at any given time.

It was found that the likelihood of falling asleep at any given moment in time can involve a number of factors including an individual’s likelihood of falling asleep on average (e.g., trait sleepiness), length of time awake, time of day, activity the individual is involved in, and posture.

Goel, Rao, Durmer & Dinges, 2009

Journal article

Public

A thorough review was conducted on the neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation.

Found a number of cognitive functions are impaired by sleep loss. These deficits occurred in psychomotor performance (vigilance and speed), response inhibition, working memory, cognitive speed, executive functions and higher cognitive functions such as decision making, focused attention and lateral thinking.

Individuals who are sleep deprived are not always aware of the severity of their impairment.

Some individuals may be genetically predisposed to the cognitive impairments associated with sleep loss.

Biological clock generates the circadian rhythm which effects sleepiness levels. Extended wakefulness also leads to sleepiness and the increased likelihood of falling asleep.

Time-on-task can lead to increased cognitive impairment, this fatigue effect is more prominent after sleep deprivation.

The cognitive impairments due to sleep loss are often highly variable both within individuals and between individuals.



Jung, Ronda, Czeisler & Wright, 2010.

Journal article

Public

Investigated the effect of sleep deprivation on sustained auditory and visual attention. Performance was measured every two hours during 40 hours of sleep deprivation.

Sleep deprivation lead to impairment in both visual and auditory attention however visual vigilance was more impaired and more variable compared to auditory vigilance. Impairments included longer response times, increased lapses (failure to respond within 10ms), inappropriate responses.

Time-on-task also increased with increasing impairment.



Johns, 2010

Journal article

Public

A review was conducted on the concepts of sleep and wakefulness including what sources of variance may lead to an individual’s likelihood of falling asleep (sleep propensity).

The three sources of variance related to an individual’s sleep propensity included average sleep propensity of that individual, the capacity for the person’s posture, activity and situation to facilitate the onset of sleep, and the way the individual responds to those particular circumstances.

Moller, Kayumov, Bulmash, Nhan & Shapiro, 2006

Journal article

Public

A study was conducted to investigate the circadian fluctuation in alertness and performance on a driving simulator in healthy individuals.

Objective measures of performance (e.g., reaction time) showed circadian variation, however subjective measures did not, suggesting a lack of awareness of some sleep-related deficits.

Micro sleeps were relatively common in the late afternoon and an increase in micro sleeps was strongly correlated with an increase in crashes.



Franzen, Siegle & Buysse, 2008

Journal article

Public

After a night of normal sleep or total sleep deprivation, a group of healthy participants completed objective and subjective measures of sleepiness as well as emotional regulation and vigilance tasks.

Sleep deprivation lead to increased subjective and objective sleepiness. After sleep deprivation the participants were more reactive to emotional stimuli, had longer reaction times and more lapses (reaction times greater than 500ms).

Roads and Traffic Authority, NSW Centre for Road Safety, 2008

Roads and Traffic Authority Report

Public

Some criteria for determining if fatigue was involved in a crash, post-crash, were reported on. Crash statistics in NSW were also included in the report.

In order for a crash to be considered fatigue related, the report states that at least one fatigued driver must have been involved in the crash. To meet these criteria the police must have suspected the driver was asleep, drowsy or fatigued, or the manoeuvre must have suggested fatigue. For example, a vehicle travelling on a straight road drifting into head-on traffic when not overtaking or travelling there on purpose for some other reason. Or, If the vehicle travelled off the side of a straight road or left the outside of a curve when excessive speed was not involved and no other reason could be identified for the manoeuvre.

Fatigue was involved in 16% of all fatal crashes and 9% of all injury crashes in NSW in 2008.

Regardless of fatigue, 0.8% of all crashes were fatal, however 2% of all fatigue related crashes were fatal in NSW in 2008.


Queensland Transport, 2008

Queensland Transport Report

Public

QLD crash statistics for the financial year of 2007-2008 were reported on.

17.5% of fatal crashes were fatigue related.

Van Dongen, Maislin, Mullington & Dinges, 2003

Journal article

Public

Using an experimental design, the effects of chronic sleep restriction and total sleep restriction on neuro-behavioural functioning was investigated.

The researchers argued that chronic sleep restriction is particularly relevant to every day life compared to total sleep restriction.

Impairments in psychomotor vigilance, working memory and cognitive throughput were found after chronically restricting sleep to 4 and 6 hours per night.

Participants did not adapt to chronic partial sleep deprivation, after 14 days, cognitive deficits were comparable to those after 1 to 2 days of total sleep deprivation.

Subjective sleepiness ratings were greater after total sleep deprivation and initially after chronic partial sleep deprivation, however, these ratings showed adaption to the chronic partial sleep deprivation. That is, after 14 days of chronic partial sleep deprivation, cognitive performance was at it’s worst, however participants reported only feeling slightly sleepy.

After chronic sleep restriction, participants spent less time in stages 1, 2, and REM sleep.

Those who naturally sleep longer may be more affected by 14 days of sleep deprivation.



Horne & Reyner, 1995

Journal article

Public

Two surveys were conducted in southwest England and the midlands using police databases and interviews to determine the time of day and prevalence of sleep related crashes. Criteria for assessing the involvement of sleep in a crash were included.

Criteria for identifying a sleep related crash included a BAC below the legal limit, no signs of braking, speeding, following too close, or mechanical defect. The weather must have allowed for clear visibility and the police officers must have suspected sleepiness as a prime cause at the scene. Finally, the vehicle was required to have run off the road or run into the back of another vehicle, and for several seconds before leaving the road or hitting the vehicle, the driver would have been able to clearly see the hazard.

In 1987 to 1992 inclusive, 16% of all crashes in which police were called in southwest England were found to be sleep related. Three peaks in sleep related crashes were found, 2-3am, 6-7am, and 4-5pm.

23% of all crashes on motorways in the midlands during August 1991 and 1992, and April 1994 were sleep related. There was a peak in sleep-related crashes between 12am-3am and during the mid-afternoon.

Sleep related crashes can occur even after a short period of driving due to the influence of the circadian rhythm.



Gander, Marshall, James & Le Quesne, 2006

Journal article

Public

An investigation of the prevalence of fatigue in truck crashes was conducted. The researchers demonstrated the difference between two different methods of determining the presence of fatigue in the crashes.

There are a number of different issues with identifying fatigue in a crash. That is, drivers may not be fully aware of their fatigue or it’s effects, there may be little evidence of fatigue symptoms at the crash scene, and often crash investigators have insufficient knowledge of fatigue in order to reliably determine it’s involvement.

Crash reports suggested only 5.1% of truck crashes in New Zealand during 2001 and 2002 involved fatigue. However, when other factors were considered, such as physiological risk factors and the driver’s opinion of fatigue involvement, 17.6% of crashes were classified as fatigue related.

Somewhere between 29-59% of fatigue related crashes may not be classified as fatigue related on crash reports.


Connor, Whitlock, Norton & Jackson, 2001

Journal article

Public

A review of international epidemiological studies investigating the involvement of sleepiness in car crashes was conducted using an assortment of cross-sectional studies and one case-control study.

The better quality studies reviewed indicated there was likely to be a positive relationship between fatigue (due to either sleep disorders, shift work, sleep deprivation, or excessive daytime sleepiness) and crash risk. Evidence for a causal role however is weak from the epidemiological evidence.

Cummings, Koepsell, Moffat & Rivara, 2001

Journal article

Public

Factors related to driver drowsiness and countermeasures were investigated in relation to crash risk using a case-control design in rural Washington State during 1997 and 1998.

Drivers who felt they were falling asleep at the wheel, those who slept equal to or less than nine hours (compared to 12 hours) in the previous 48 hours, and drivers who drove longer distances were at greater risk of crash. Those who used rest stops, drank coffee within the preceding two hours, or used their radios were at less risk of crash.

By stopping driving when drivers are fighting sleep, using highway rest stops, drinking coffee, using the radio, getting at least nine hours sleep in the 48 hours prior to a trip, and avoiding long distances or sharing driving may reduce risk of crash.



Connor et al., 2002

Journal article

Public

Using a case-control design, the contribution of sleepiness to serious injury crashes was investigated in New Zealand during 1998 to 1999. Factors leading to increased risk of crash were identified.

Greater acute sleepiness was related to greater risk of crash.

Drivers who felt they were sleepy, reported less than five hours sleep (compared to those reporting more than five hours) in the preceding 24 hours, and those driving between 2-5am were at greater risk of crash.

Chronic sleepiness was not associated with an increase in crash risk.


Fell & Black, 1997.

Journal article

Public

A telephone survey investigating the relationship between fatigue and crashes in metropolitan areas was conducted in the region of Sydney in 1995. Information from both crashes and near-crashes were included in the survey.

27% of drivers involved in a fatigue-related crash or near-crash reported they had not felt tired at the start of their trip, despite this they all acknowledged that the crash was due to fatigue.

Risk factors for a fatigue related incident may be due to tiredness due to sleep loss, late night driving, and shift-working.

City fatigue related driving incidents tended to occur on work trips or commuting to and from work, as well as social trips.


Quarck, Ventre, Etard & Denise, 2006

Journal article

Public

A within-subjects experimental design was used to determine the effects of 26-29 hours of sleep deprivation on the vestibular-ocular-reflex, a measure of vestibular functioning.

A change in the vestibular-ocular reflex was found after sleep deprivation. The researchers suggested that the related impairment in vestibular functioning may lead sleep deprived individuals to misperceive their own body’s movement in space.

Swann, Yelland, Redman & Rajaratnam, 2006

Journal article

Public

The researchers used event related potentials to determine the effects of partial sleep deprivation on automatic and selective attention.

The findings of the study suggested that sleep partial sleep deprivation can lead to impairment in the ability to automatically detect change and that the brain recruits more resources to sustain selective attention while sleep deprived.

Drummond, Paulus & Tapert, 2006

Journal article

Public

The researchers investigated the effects of two nights of consecutive sleep deprivation on participants ability to inhibit responses. Recovery from this impairment was also investigated with two nights of recovery sleep.

The study found both one and two nights of total sleep deprivation lead to an impairment in ability to inhibit inappropriate responses.

By the second night of sleep deprivation participants produced increased errors of omission

Both of these impairments returned to normal after one night of recovery sleep


Belenky et al., 2003

Journal article

Public

The effects of either three, five, seven, or nine hours of partial sleep deprivation over seven days on psychomotor performance was evaluated. The recovery of performance was also measures over three days of recovery sleep (eight hours in bed over night each night).

The researchers argued that studies of chronic partial sleep deprivation are more relevant to every day life, compared to total sleep deprivation, because this is more likely to occur outside the laboratory.

During sleep deprivation, speed and lapses remained at baseline levels for the nine hour group. For the seven hour group there was an initial reduction in psychomotor speed which then stabilised at a slowed rate. The effect was similar for the five hour sleep restriction group however this group also experienced greater numbers of psychomotor lapses. The three hour group had increasingly slower reaction times and greater numbers of lapses over the seven days of sleep restriction.

During recovery sleep, there was no evidence of recovery found in the five or seven hour sleep restriction group. Impairment in speed and lapses in the three hour group recovered after one night of recovery sleep however they did not recover to baseline levels, rather they stabilised at a level of impairment similar to the five and seven hour group.

Recovery from chronic partial sleep restriction is not as rapid as that of total sleep deprivation.



Lamond & Dawson, 1999

Journal article

Public

The researchers compared the effects of sleep deprivation to that of alcohol intoxication using a number of tasks including simple sensory comparison, unpredictable tracking, vigilance (accuracy and latency), and grammatical reasoning (accuracy and latency).

28 hours of sustained wakefulness lead to impairment on all tasks other than the accuracy of grammatical reasoning and the simple sensory task.

The more complex tasks in the study were more sensitive to the effects of fatigue compared to the relatively simpler tasks.

After 20 hours of sustained wakefulness, performance impairment was equivalent to a BAC of 0.10%.


Maruff, Falleti, Collie, Darby & McStephen, 2005

Journal article

Public

The researchers extended the work conducted by other researchers on the relative effects of sleep deprivation and alcohol on performance. They suggested their design to be more accurate as they accounted for changes in the variability of data.

The researchers argued that previous studies overestimated the effect of fatigue on performance because they did not take into account changes in performance variability.

Performance impairment after 24 hours of sustained wakefulness corresponded with the impairments found at a BAC of 0.05.

Increased reaction times were found with sustained wakefulness, while performance on psychomotor tasks also increased in variability,


Lim & Dinges, 2010

Journal article

Public

A meta-analysis of seventy previous studies was conducted in order to investigate the impact of sleep deprivation on a number of cognitive variables including simple attention, complex attention, working memory, processing speed, short-term memory, and reasoning.

Complex tasks were found to be less sensitive to sleep deprivation compared to simpler tasks.

Sleep deprivation impaired performance in most cognitive domains.

Simple attention and vigilance was most affected by sleep deprivation.

Complex attention and working memory tasks were only moderately affected by sleep deprivation.

Accuracy of reasoning and crystallised intelligence were not influenced by sleep deprivation.

Sleep deprivation differentially effects the various cognitive domains but does not bias people to respond faster or more accurately.



Urrila, Stenuit, Huhdankoski, Kerkhofs & Porkka-Heiskanen, 2007

Journal article

Public

The researchers investigated the effects of age on 40 hours of total sleep deprivation related performance impairment in women.

Sleep deprivation lead to impairment in psychomotor vigilance.

Age did not influence this impairment.



Philip et al., 2004

Journal article

Public

The researchers compared the performance of a younger (20-25 years) and older age group (52-63 years) on a reaction time task after a night of sleep deprivation and after a night of sleep.

After a night of sleep the older participants produced slower reaction times compared to the younger participants.

After a night of sleep deprivation the younger participants produced slower reaction times but the older participants’ reaction times remained unaffected.



Otmani, Roge & Muzet

Journal article

Public

The researchers investigated the effects of age and time of day on sleepiness ratings in professional drivers. The study used a driving simulator task during the afternoon and evening and subjective and objective sleepiness was measured during the tasks.

The younger drivers experienced greater decreases in alertness during the driving tasks compared to middle-aged drivers. The levels of sleepiness reported were greater in the younger group both during and after the driving tasks.

There was no difference found in objective sleepiness measures during the driving task between the groups.

Both subjective and objective sleepiness measures showed the drivers were less alert and more sleepy during the evening compared to the afternoon simulated driving session.

With less traffic during the simulated tasks there was greater objective sleepiness.



Mortazavi, Eskandarian & Sayed, 2009

Journal article

Public

The relationship between drowsiness and performance on a truck simulator in commercial vehicle drivers was investigated. The simulated scenario involved a monotonous section of highway and was completed by the drivers during the morning (commencing 8:30-9:30am) and at night (between 1:30am-5:00am).

Greater drowsiness lead to impairment in lane keeping and steering control

Crashes seemed to be preceded by two phases of changes in steering wheel use behaviour. Firstly, lane keeping and steering control variables were affected. The second phase involved ‘dosing off’ in which the steering angle was constant and there was no input from the driver. Run off road crashes were associated with this latter phase.

There are individual differences which suggest drowsiness detection systems based on changes in steering wheel behaviour may fail to issue warnings for some drivers or in some situations, whereas in other cases they may give false alarms.


Charlton & Baas, 2001

Journal article

Public

The relationship between fatigue, work/rest cycles and performance (psychomotor and driving simulator) was conducted in 606 truck drivers. The tests were conducted at truck stops, depots and ferry terminals in New Zealand.

Truck drivers worked five days per week on average with most shifts averaging about 11 hours.

The truck drivers reported an average of approximately 7 hours sleep in the previous 24 hours.

Fatigue was considered a greater problem for other drivers than themselves and only 63% of the truck drivers stated that fatigue was ‘always’ dangerous for drivers.

The researchers suggested that older drivers may be more susceptible to fatigue related impairment

Age and length of prior rest/sleep predicted failure rates on the driving simulator.

The researchers did not directly study this but suggested professional drivers may be less susceptible to fatigue impairment due to their greater driving experience.

The researchers suggested at the time of the study, the current regulations for service hours were not effective in managing fatigue or driver compliance.


Akerstedt, Peters, Anund & Kecklund, 2005

Journal article

Public

The relationship between driving performance and sleepiness after a night shit was conducted using a driving simulator in shift workers. Driving performance was measured after a night shift and after a normal night of sleep.

Lane position variability and number of incidents increased while the time to first accident decreased after night shift compared to after a night of sleep. The duration of eye closure was longer and subjective sleepiness also increased after night shift compared to after a normal night of sleep.

Boyle, Tippin, Paul & Rizzo, 2008

Journal article

Public

The performance impairment on a driving simulator during micro-sleeps in a group of drivers with sleep apnoea was compared to performance outside of micro-sleeps.

Driving performance during micro-sleep episodes was found to be reduced compared to periods of wakefulness. This reduced performance was related to both the duration and occurrence of micro-sleeps.

Schmidt, Schrauf, Simon, Fritzsche, Buchner & Kincses, 2009

Journal article

Public

A simulated monotonous daytime driving scenario of 428km was used to assess drivers’ subjective and objective state of vigilance and how this related to the monotonous driving task.

There was a continuous reduction in objective vigilance found over the 428kms, however, subjective vigilance followed this trend until the final section of the drive. At this stage, the subjective and objective measures of vigilance did not equate, with subjective ratings of vigilance improving while objective measures continued to deteriorate.

The researchers suggested the knowledge that a monotonous trip is soon to be completed may make drivers feel their vigilance levels have improved while their actual state of vigilance continues to become increasingly impaired with continued driving.



Davey, Richards & Freeman, 2007

Journal article

Public

A study was conducted in order to determine the patterns of use and reasons for illicit drug use among long-distance truck drivers.

There were a number of different reasons for drug use in the truck drivers, one of the main reasons was in order to combat fatigue.

The most common illicit drug used by the drivers was amphetamines.



Oron-Gilad & Ronen, 2007

Journal article

Public

The researchers sort to determine the influence of road characteristics (such as curved, straight and mixed roads) on fatigue-related performance in a driving simulator.

Driving is a fatigue inducing task, that is, drivers can experience fatigue early in a drive even when they are not tired or sleep deprived.

Fatigue symptoms show large individual differences

Driving performance impairments due to fatigue were found to relate to the road environment, that is, not the same impairments were found in curved and straight roads.


Fournier, Montreuil & Brun, 2007

Journal article

Public

Observations of both experienced and inexperienced truck drivers while working were used to determine the differences of these individuals in fatigue management and implicate possible areas for improvement in truck driver training.

Experienced drivers may have developed skills to manage their work demands as a whole, not simply via basic time management but also by being aware of changes in their own psychological and physical state and by continuously re-evaluating their working situation, and therefore may be better equipped to manage their own fatigue compared to inexperienced drivers.

By monitoring and managing their own state, as well as actively avoiding situations which can lead to stress, may lead to slower fatigue development. Continuously re-evaluating situations may also be able to aid in this way, as well as allowing for better time management to allow for rest breaks and avoid feeling pressure to drive while fatigued to make up lost time.

Inexperienced drivers may be so preoccupied with deadlines that they may not be in a psychological state which allows them to re-evaluate the situation compared to experienced drivers.

Fatigue-related driver training may benefit from including, not only basic time management principles, but also relevant work-related planning in the context of issues that often occur in the daily life of truck drivers on the job.



Woods & Grandin, 2008

Journal article

Public

Using accident reports of commercial livestock truck crashes between 1994 and 2007 in the US and Canada, the involvement of fatigue was investigated.

The researchers suggested that a large proportion of livestock truck crashes are due to fatigue because 59% of the crashes occurred between 12:00am and 9:00am and the majority were single vehicle crashes (80%). In addition, 85% of the crashes were considered due to an error on behalf of the truck driver.

Heaton, Browning & Anderson, 2008

Journal article

Public

Using a logistic regression analysis, the researchers attempted to determine which variables can predict falling asleep while driving in truck drivers. Demographic variables, sleep-related variables, and the Epworth Sleepiness Scale was used.

Four variables were found to predict falling asleep at the wheel within the previous 30 days, these included, an Epworth Sleepiness Scale score over 10, greater than six hours night-time driving duration, working more than 13 hours in a 24-hour period, and using medications related to sleep and wakefulness.

An ESS score greater than 10 lead drivers to be at three times greater the risk of falling asleep at the wheel.

Those truck drivers who reported greater than six hours night-time driving were four times as likely to fall asleep at the wheel compared to those reporting fewer hours.

Working more than 13 hours lead to 2.5 times the risk of falling asleep while driving compared to those who worked less than 13 hours in a 24-hour period.

Those truck drivers using medications were nearly five times more likely to fall asleep while driving.

Years of experience was not related to an increase in risk of falling asleep, nor was driving solo compared to driving with a partner at least 50% of the time.



Duke, Guest & Boggess, 2010

Journal article

Public

A literature review was conducted to determine the relationship between age and crashes/safety in professional heavy vehicle drivers.

The researchers focused on age-related safety however, they also reported that long hours and related sleepiness and fatigue contributed to heavy vehicle crashes.

The researchers noted that there is inconsistent evidence on the age-related effects of fatigue on crash risk, however larger studies suggest that younger drivers may be at greater risk, suggesting that younger drivers may be more suited to short haul driving.



Milia, Smolensky, Costa, Howarth, Ohayon & Philip, 2011

Journal article

Public

The researchers conducted a review of both endogenous and exogenous variables that can potentially lead to fatigue and/or the recognition and response to it on behalf of individuals.

The researchers considered a number of variables in their review which they commented on.

Endogenous variables with the potential to influence fatigue included: genetic factors, gender, age, race, nutrition, BMI, endurance (both mental and physical), circadian strength, chronotype, phase and desynchrony, personality, sleep requirement and debt, and health status (physical and psychological).

Exogenous variables with the potential to influence fatigue included: Working arrangements, time and method of commuting, physical and cognitive state at commencement of shift, the start time and duration of the shift, workload, motivation, time since last sleep, quality and duration of sleep, napping, recovery time between shifts, meal timing and content, work conditions, medication and drug use, job control, monotony, and so on.


Department for Transport, Energy and Infrastructure (DTEI), 2010

DTEI report

Public

An investigation of the prevalence of heavy vehicle crashes in South Australia and the relationship between these crashes and a number of variables, including fatigue, were presented in the report.

In SA during 2005-2009, 17% of fatal heavy vehicle crashes involved fatigue.

More articulated compared to non-articulated trucks were involved in fatigue related crashes.

Inconsistent definitions of fatigue makes the involvement of fatigue in crashes difficult to determine.

The potential role of fatigue can also be difficult to determine post-crash.



Klauer, Dingus & Neale, 2009

Symposium proceedings

Public

The differential effects of fatigue and driving performance for single and team long-haul truck drivers were compared in a naturalistic study.

Solo drivers were four times more at risk of drowsiness related incidents compared to team drivers. The researchers suggested this was because team drivers were more likely to change driving duties prior to excessive fatigue. In comparison, solo drivers were more likely to continue driving while fatigued.

Moore-Ede, Heitmann, Guttkuhn, Trutschel, Aguirre & Croke, 2004

Journal article

Public

The application of the Circadian Alertness Simulator or CAS (a mathematical model designed to predict fatigue) was evaluated by the researchers within the field of trucking.

The researchers argued that the CAS was effective in predicting fatigue in the trucking application. By providing managers with the CAS, these managers could then make informed decisions on fatigue risk, the differences in decisions made based on the CAS lead to significantly reduced crash rate and severity of heavy truck crashes.

Dijk & Larkin, 2004

Journal article

Public

The authors commented on the theoretical basis behind current mathematical models of fatigue prediction (including the CAS) and suggested areas for future research.

The researchers suggested that the theoretical basis behind mathematical models of fatigue prediction need to be re-evaluated, with most models using relatively simplistic algorithms with minimal variables.

Furthermore, the assumptions underlying the models in terms of the relationship between these variables, other sleep-related variables and performance requires further validation.

The researchers noted that the ability for the CAS to use individual data in predictions is commendable.


Anund & Kircher, 2009

VTI report

Public

The authors investigated the evaluation of warning strategies in fatigue-detection systems and commented on the different possibilities and their pros and cons, including laboratory evaluations and naturalistic studies.

The development of fatigue detection technologies has received much research attention but the strategies of warning drivers has received little attention in comparison.

Evaluating warning strategies is important because if a fatigued driver does not respond appropriately to the warning, there is no safety benefit in using the technology.

Evaluation of warning strategies should be conducted in different contexts, laboratory studies provide the ability for researchers to control confounding variables, but they may lead to differing results compared to naturalistic settings. Therefore, despite the disadvantage of lack of control, naturalistic studies are also highly important.


Haworth, Heffernan & Horne, 1989

MUARC report

Public

The researchers assessed the involvement of fatigue in fatal truck crashes and reviewed fatigue countermeasures with a particular focus on in-vehicle devices.

19.9% of fatal crashes involving trucks between 1984 and 1986 were judged as being associated with fatigue by the researchers.

In-vehicle countermeasures were considered superior to on-road countermeasures as they allow rapid detection of any sudden changes in alertness and they operate constantly.

On-road countermeasures such as rumble strips may be effective but they are relatively expensive so their deployment can only be in specific high crash locations (therefore their benefit is not constant throughout a trip).

In-vehicle countermeasures could be dangerous if drivers rely too heavily on these devices to alert them of when their driving is impaired.

Analysis of steering patterns may be particularly valid compared to other measures while eye closure and head nodding measures may also have potential.

Driver strategies such as playing alerting games need to be further evaluated.

There is little evidence that introducing cold air to the driving environment will reduce fatigue, that is, unless the driving environment is particularly hot.

An effective system will rarely produce false alarms, or give warning too late, it will not be overly intrusive and should be sensitive to all levels of fatigue (not simply extreme fatigue)



Balkin, Horrey, Graeber, Czeisler & Dinges, 2011

Journal article

Public

A comprehensive review was conducted of fatigue detection technologies, related issues, and directions for future research in the field.

The researchers listed criteria for an ideal fatigue management system including the ability to predict fatigue, measure and monitor fatigue and intervene in order to sustain alertness.

Using individualised data is also beneficial in fatigue detection technologies.

Challenges for the technologies were considered including the ratio of misses and false alarms and intrusiveness.

Operator compliance and reliance are also issues which technologies must overcome.



Caterpillar, 2008.

Caterpillar report

Public

A detailed review on currently available fatigue detection technologies was conducted. 22 technologies were evaluated and rated in comparison to each other based on a number of categories. The pros and cons of the technologies were discussed. The review was based on the mining industry.

The top five technologies were ASTid (Pernix), FaceLab (Seeing Machines), HaulCheck (Accumine), Optalert (Sleep Diagnostics) and the Driver State Monitor (Delphi),

Head nodding technologies received very low scores due to numerous false alarms and misses.

ASTiD and Optalert were the only two technologies recommended by the researchers for fatigue detection.


Friswell, Williamson, & Dunn (2006)

Injury Risk Management Research Centre (UNSW) Report

Public

Compares the fatigue experiences of 1007 long distance HV drivers with 321 short haul truck drivers.

Effects of fatigue were similar for short haul and long distance drivers in terms of reported safety incidents and personal experiences of fatigue.

There were clear differences in the causes of fatigue for the two types of driver. Short haul drivers worked long hours with a significant number of deliveries and pick ups, and had to deal with heavy urban traffic. Long haul drivers also worked long hours, however spent more time waiting for loading and unloading, and participated in more monotonous rural driving.

The nature of fatigue-related incidents is determined by the demands of the driving environment. Rural and urban environments present different demands.

Short haul drivers were less likely to view fatigue as a problem for the industry than were long haul drivers.

There is a need to reduce fatigue causing factors within the short and long haul transport sectors. There is also a need to raise awareness of driver fatigue issues for short haul drivers.

Table 5.13


Sleep apnoea and related performance impairment and crashes

Authors

Type

Availability

Research

Findings

Pizza, Contardi, Ferlisi, Mondini & Cirignotta, 2008

Journal article

Public

Results of subjective and objective measures of sleepiness were related to performance on a driving simulator task by patients with sleep apnoea.

Increased objective and subjective sleepiness related to poorer performance on the driving simulator (shown in increased crashes and variability in lane position).

Sleep apnoea patients were aware of their sleepiness and related driving impairment.



Pizza, Contardi, Mondini, Trentin & Cirignotta, 2009

Journal article

Public

Objective and subjective measures of sleepiness were taken and a driving simulation task was undertaken by patients with severe sleep apnoea.

Sleep-related crashes are not only due to falling asleep but also from impairments caused by sleepiness itself. In line with this, impairments in driving performance on simulator were more closely related to more general objective measures of sleepiness than those measuring patients ability to remain awake.

Tregear, Reston, Schoelles & Phillips, 2009

Journal article

Public

A review and meta-analysis of the risk of crash associated with sleep apnoea was conducted. The review focused on commercial motor vehicle drivers. The researchers also attempted to determine what factors lead to greater risk of crash within drivers diagnosed with sleep apnoea.

Sleep apnoea is particularly prevalent in commercial motor vehicle drivers.

Drivers with sleep apnoea were found to be at increased risk of crash compared to those who do not have the disorder.

Drivers with sleep apnoea who may be at particular risk are those with high body mass indexes, hypoxemia, greater severity of disordered breathing during sleep, and greater daytime sleepiness.


Moreno, Louzada, Teixeira, Borges & Lorenzi-Filho, 2006

Journal article

Public

Investigated the relationship between body weight and sleep patterns in truck drivers.

The researchers found that short-sleep durations are common among truck drivers because of irregular work shifts and that this decrease in sleep duration is associated with greater BMI.

Obesity was associated with snoring.

Truck drivers often have a poor diet and tend to be sedentary in their activities.

High BMI was associated with risk of sleep apnoea



Teran-Santos, Jimenez-Gomez, Cordero-Guevara & Burgos-Santander, 1999

Journal article

Public

A case-control design was used to assess the risk of crash associated with sleep apnoea. Participants in the ‘case’ group were those who received emergency treatment due to a crash on Spanish highways between April and December 1995.

Sleep apnoea was strongly related to crashes, with those with sleep apnoea having a greater likelihood of crash.

Even small quantities of alcohol taken on the day of the crash intensified the relationship between sleep apnoea and crashes.



Pierce, 1999

Journal article

Public

This article considered issues associated with driver sleepiness, including the causes of sleepiness and the driving-related risk associated with sleep apnoea.

Sleepiness is involved in approximately 30% of crashes.

Shift work, poor quality and insufficient sleep, medications and medical conditions (including sleep apnoea) can lead to excessive sleepiness.

There is a relationship between sleep apnoea and crashes.

Sleep apnoea, regardless of the symptoms of excessive sleepiness, is related to an increase in risk of crash.



Charlton et al., 2004

MUARC report

Public

A comprehensive review of the relationship between chronic medical conditions and crashes was conducted. Sleep apnoea was considered among a number of other conditions.

Sleep apnoea is associated with an increased risk of car crash, largely due to falling asleep at the wheel.

The severity of sleep apnoea influences the extent the condition increases crash risk, more severe apnoea leads to greater risk.

It is important to identify the differences between those with sleep apnoea who have crashes and those that do not.

CPAP therapy for sleep apnoea tends to reduce the risk of crash to that of healthy controls.



Smolensky, Milia, Ohayon & Philip, 2011

Journal article

Public

A comprehensive review of previously published literature on the relationship between sleep disorders (including sleep apnoea), medical conditions, and crash risk was conducted. Comments on the effectiveness of treatments and suggestions for the focus of future research in this field were also included.

Previous research on sleep disorders and traffic crashes have largely focused on sleep apnoea.

The prevalence of sleep apnoea is likely to be much greater among professional drivers compared to the general population.

Studies on sleep apnoea and crash risk largely confirm sleep apnoea is related to increased crash risk in both commercial and non-commercial drivers.

Many studies investigating sleep apnoea and crashes fail to assess and evaluate the relative role of other potentially confounding variables including other medical conditions, medication use, and demographics in crash risk.

Therapies for sleep apnoea including CPAP, UPPP, and OApps may be effective in reducing risk of crash due to fatigue in sleep apnoea patients. CPAP, however, has not been shown to be useful in all patients. There may be a role for medications in the treatment of patients with sleep apnoea who do not respond to CPAP treatment.

Further research is required on the effectiveness and cost-benefit ratio of different therapies and treatments, particularly because CPAP has been largely focused on in the literature.

The relationship between other sleep disorders and medical conditions that lead to fatigue have been relatively ignored in the literature (compared to sleep apnoea), future research should also focus on determining the involvement of these disorders and conditions in crashes, and the effectiveness of treatments in reducing crash risk. Such conditions include, narcolepsy, hypersomnia, periodic limb movement disorders, restless legs syndrome, rhinitis, asthma, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, arthritis and chronic fatigue syndrome.

The researchers also noted that the varied definitions and methods of measuring fatigue make the findings of research on fatigue difficult to compare.



Table 5.14
Substance use

Author

Type

Availability

Research

Findings

Potter (2005)

Conference paper

Public

An overview of Australian approaches to police drug and alcohol enforcement for drivers. Emphasis is placed on commercial vehicle operations.

A major component of police strategy has been a deterrence based approach to enforcement rather than mandatory workplace testing or for cause testing.

Discusses the use of roadside testing procedures to reduce the incidence of drug and alcohol use by drivers.

Compares the advantages and limitations of methods used in Australia and in other countries.


Couper, Pemberton, Jarvis, Hughes, & Logan

Journal article

Public

Reports the prevalence of drug use among commercial truck drivers based on assessments of 1079 drivers, 822 of whom provided anonymous urine specimens. The study was undertaken in the United States.

21% of urine specimens tested positive for either illicit, prescription, or over the counter drugs; 7% tested positive for more than one drug.

The largest number of positive findings (9.5%) were for stimulants such as methamphetamine, amphetamines, ephedrine/pseudoephedrine, and cocaine.

The second most common drug was cannabis (4.3%).

1.3% of drivers tested positive for alcohol.



Leyton, et al. (2011)

Journal article

Public

Reports the prevalence of drug use among truck drivers in Brazil. Of 488 drivers stopped at random 456 provided urine samples which were screened for drugs.

9.3% tested positive for drugs. Of these 61% were amphetamines,

25% were cocaine, and 12% were cannabinoids.




Mabbott & Hartley (1999)

Journal article

Public

A study of the use of stimulant drug use amongst 236 truck drivers interviewed in Western Australia.

27% of drivers reported using stimulant drugs to combat driver fatigue.

Interstate driver use more prescription and illicit drugs to stay awake while intrastate drivers rely more on over the counter medications.;

The most frequent methods for obtaining stimulant drugs were through a doctor, a chemist, or illegal prescription.


Williamson (2007)

Journal article

Public

An analysis of truck driver substance use to determine the predictors of stimulant drug use. Interview data collected from 970 drivers in 1991 and 1007 drivers in 2001 was used for the present study.

20-33% of truck drivers reported using stimulants at least sometimes.

A significant proportion of drivers reported stimulant use as a helpful fatigue management strategy.

Drivers who had the greatest problem managing fatigue were twice as likely to use stimulants.

Drivers paid on a payment-by-results or contingency payment basis were 2-3 times more likely to use stimulants.

Younger, less experienced drivers were also more likely to use drugs.

This study demonstrates the influence of external factors, particularly productivity-based payment systems, on the stimulant drug use of truck drivers.



Richards (2005)

Thesis (Masters)

Public

Used qualitative data from 35 long haul truck drivers to better understand the substance using behaviours of truck drivers.

High rates of licit and illicit drug (particularly amphetamines) use were reported. (However the sample size for this study is rather small to generalise these findings to all heavy vehicle drivers).

Some drivers begin using drugs before they begin driving trucks.

Apart from fatigue motivations for drug use included peer pressure, socialisation, relaxation, addiction, and wanting to fit the trucking “image”.

1   ...   5   6   7   8   9   10   11   12   ...   16


Verilənlər bazası müəlliflik hüququ ilə müdafiə olunur ©atelim.com 2016
rəhbərliyinə müraciət